Biologist: The evolutionary advantage of sexual reproduction is that it increases the range of genetic variation in a species, which is an advantage for the species as a whole. However, an increased range of genetic variation in a species is not advantageous for any individual member of the species. It follows that the sole reason that sexual reproduction has become the rule among both animals and plants is that natural selection has favored some entire species over others.

Summarize Argument
The biologist argues that the evolutionary advantage of sexual reproduction is increasing genetic variation. But this benefit applies to the species as a whole, not to individual members. Thus, the only reason that sexual reproduction is widespread among animals and plants is because natural selection has favored entire species over others.

Identify Argument Part
This is a premise that is used to support the author’s conclusion that natural selection has favored some species over others.

A
It is a claim offered in support of the argument’s conclusion.
This is an accurate description of the claim. It directly supports the conclusion that the reason sexual reproduction is prevalent is due to natural selection favoring some species over others.
B
It is presented to raise a question that sheds doubt on the argument’s conclusion.
This statement does not cast doubt on the conclusion. It is the main premise used to support it.
C
It is a claim that the argument is designed to call into question.
The argument does not challenge this claim. The biologist believes this claim and uses it as a premise to support the conclusion
D
It describes an observed phenomenon for which the argument seeks an explanation.
The argument does not seek to explain this claim. The claim is used as support to explain the main conclusion.
E
It is presented as the main explanation of the origin of an observed phenomenon.
This statement is not the main explanation for the observed phenomenon. The “main explanation” of the phenomenon is the conclusion of the argument.

3 comments

Great question! I was tempted by (C) as well and had it selected for a hot second when I took this section.

(C) is wrong because while it's descriptively accurate, it doesn't describe the vulnerability in the reasoning. It's true that the columnist failed to specify what is meant by "innocuous everyday occurrences." But that's not why the argument is vulnerable.

This kind of wrong answer tends to be harder to spot than the descriptively inaccurate answers. But, we can see this by "fixing this mistake." Let's go ahead and "improve" the argument by specifying the what these "innocuous everyday occurrences" are.

Here's the new and "improved" argument:

Columnist: Vagrancy laws are supposed to reduce criminal activity, but they don't. Making vagrancy illegal means transforming many innocuous everyday occurrences, e.g. sleeping on the sidewalk, roaming aimlessly around the neighborhood, etc., into crimes. Thus, vagrancy laws increase crime while purporting to reduce it.

What do you think? Is the argument actually improved? Hardly. Barely. The conclusion still doesn't follow from the premises. It's still as weakly supported as before we specified what "innocuous everyday occurrences" are.

Why? (E) tells us why.

(E) could also have been written as a more "cookie cutter" flaw: shift in meaning of a key term, phrase, or concept. At the start of the argument, what do you think "reduce criminal activity" means? What kinds of "criminal activity" do you think vagrancy laws are "supposed to reduce?" Probably mostly low level crimes like drug use, petty theft or other property crimes, and okay maybe some more serious crimes. (Put aside your thoughts on the ethics of vagrancy laws, that's not the point here.) Now look at the conclusion where it says vagrancy laws "increase crime." In what sense do vagrancy laws increase crime? Do they increase drug use, theft, etc? No. It "increases crime" in a statistical, counting, classification, categorical sense. What used to be not a crime (being a vagrant) now is a crime. Okay, so "crime has increased" in the sense of reclassifying something as a crime that didn't used to be a crime. But wait, the argument was trying to disprove the claim that vagrancy laws would reduce crimes like drug use, theft, etc.

(E) could have said "allows a key concept to illicitly shift in meaning." In other words, conflating two very different ideas. Or it could have said what it in fact said "doesn't adequately distinguish between an increase in criminal activity (idea 1) and the reclassification of certain occurrences as crimes (idea 2)."

This is a terrible weakness in reasoning. We can expose this weakness more starkly with the following argument that exhibits the same:

Homicide laws are supposed to reduce criminal activity, but they in fact have the opposite effect. Last year alone, there were 239 homicides in our city, representing 20% of all serious crimes. If we had passed the legislation I introduced to strike all homicide laws from our criminal codes, there would have been 239 fewer crimes.

Yes, there would have been 239 fewer crimes. But no, crime would not have been reduced. And that's not a contradiction because the meaning of "crime" is shifting across the two sentences.


5 comments

Great question! I was tempted by (C) as well and had it selected for a hot second when I took this section.

(C) is wrong because while it's descriptively accurate, it doesn't describe the vulnerability in the reasoning. It's true that the columnist failed to specify what is meant by "innocuous everyday occurrences." But that's not why the argument is vulnerable.

This kind of wrong answer tends to be harder to spot than the descriptively inaccurate answers. But, we can see this by "fixing this mistake." Let's go ahead and "improve" the argument by specifying the what these "innocuous everyday occurrences" are.

Here's the new and "improved" argument:

Columnist: Vagrancy laws are supposed to reduce criminal activity, but they don't. Making vagrancy illegal means transforming many innocuous everyday occurrences, e.g. sleeping on the sidewalk, roaming aimlessly around the neighborhood, etc., into crimes. Thus, vagrancy laws increase crime while purporting to reduce it.

What do you think? Is the argument actually improved? Hardly. Barely. The conclusion still doesn't follow from the premises. It's still as weakly supported as before we specified what "innocuous everyday occurrences" are.

Why? (E) tells us why.

(E) could also have been written as a more "cookie cutter" flaw: shift in meaning of a key term, phrase, or concept. At the start of the argument, what do you think "reduce criminal activity" means? What kinds of "criminal activity" do you think vagrancy laws are "supposed to reduce?" Probably mostly low level crimes like drug use, petty theft or other property crimes, and okay maybe some more serious crimes. (Put aside your thoughts on the ethics of vagrancy laws, that's not the point here.) Now look at the conclusion where it says vagrancy laws "increase crime." In what sense do vagrancy laws increase crime? Do they increase drug use, theft, etc? No. It "increases crime" in a statistical, counting, classification, categorical sense. What used to be not a crime (being a vagrant) now is a crime. Okay, so "crime has increased" in the sense of reclassifying something as a crime that didn't used to be a crime. But wait, the argument was trying to disprove the claim that vagrancy laws would reduce crimes like drug use, theft, etc.

(E) could have said "allows a key concept to illicitly shift in meaning." In other words, conflating two very different ideas. Or it could have said what it in fact said "doesn't adequately distinguish between an increase in criminal activity (idea 1) and the reclassification of certain occurrences as crimes (idea 2)."

This is a terrible weakness in reasoning. We can expose this weakness more starkly with the following argument that exhibits the same:

Homicide laws are supposed to reduce criminal activity, but they in fact have the opposite effect. Last year alone, there were 239 homicides in our city, representing 20% of all serious crimes. If we had passed the legislation I introduced to strike all homicide laws from our criminal codes, there would have been 239 fewer crimes.

Yes, there would have been 239 fewer crimes. But no, crime would not have been reduced. And that's not a contradiction because the meaning of "crime" is shifting across the two sentences.


5 comments

Future overall demand for professors can be predicted with reasonable accuracy from current birth rates. But the accuracy of predictions of future demand for music professors is lower, and that for jazz studies professors lower still.

Summary
Future overall demand for professors can be predicted with reasonable accuracy using current birth rates. However, accuracy is lower for the future demand of music professors, and even lower for the future demand of jazz studies professors.

Strongly Supported Conclusions
The more general a prediction, the more accurate that prediction is.

A
The more detailed and complete the evidence, the more precise predictions based on that evidence are.
This answer is unsupported. In the stimulus all three predictions for the demand for professors is based on the same data and yet all three predictions have different levels of accuracy.
B
The more general the level of a prediction, the more accurate it is.
This answer is strongly supported. There is an inverse correlation between a prediction’s detail and that prediction’s accuracy. Professors overall accompanied the most accurate prediction, while jazz studies professors specifically accompanied the least accurate prediction.
C
Predicting future trends becomes more difficult as the events those predictions concern become more temporally remote.
This answer is unsupported. The stimulus makes no mention of passage of time. In fact, all three predictions in the stimulus are made from current birth rates.
D
The more detailed and complete the evidence, the more confidence we can have in predictions based on that evidence.
This answer is anti-supported. In the stimulus all three predictions for the demand for professors is based on the same data and yet all three predictions have different levels of accuracy.
E
Predictions based only on general trends are unlikely to be accurate.
This answer is anti-supported. In the stimulus, the prediction regarding future overall demand for professors is reasonably accurate even while being based on general trends in current birth rates.

4 comments

Taylorism, the early-twentieth-century industrial efficiency movement pioneered by Frederick Taylor, has had a profound effect on industrialized societies. Increased productivity resulting from greater efficiency has led to increases in most workers’ standards of living. At the same time, Taylor’s methods have tended to make these workers’ jobs routine and repetitive, thereby robbing their work of some of the intrinsic worth it possessed when it emphasized creativity and talent.

Summary
Taylorism has had a profound effect on industrialized societies. Greater efficiency caused increased productivity, which in turn caused an increase in most workers’ standards of living. At the same time, Taylorism has caused these workers’ jobs to become routine and repetitive, robbing their work of some intrinsic worth it possessed when it emphasized creativity and talent.

Strongly Supported Conclusions
Some industrial developments can both benefit and harm workers.

A
Sometimes measures that appear to promote efficiency are actually deleterious to it.
This answer is anti-supported. The stimulus tells us that Taylorism caused greater efficiency.
B
Some developments in industry can benefit workers financially while making their work less rewarding in other respects.
This answer is strongly supported. Taylorism benefitted workers financially by increasing most workers’ standards of living. However, at the same time, Taylorism harmed workers by causing their work to be more routine and repetitive.
C
Increased efficiency in industrial settings is sometimes accompanied by decreases in the amount of effort workers devote to their jobs.
This answer is unsupported. The stimulus does not tell us anything about the effort workers put into their jobs. We cannot assume that just because jobs have become routine and repetitive that workers put in less effort.
D
An increase in workers’ standards of living will likely involve an accompanying decrease in how much they value their jobs overall.
This answer is unsupported. The stimulus does not give us any information about how much workers value their jobs. We cannot assume that just because jobs have become routine and repetitive that workers value their jobs less.
E
Workers who have greater than average creativity and talent are sometimes less efficient than workers who have average creativity and talent.
This answer is unsupported. The stimulus does not give us any information to determine what type of workers are more efficient than other types.

6 comments